Catálogo de publicaciones - revistas
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research
Resumen/Descripción – provisto por la editorial
No disponible.
Palabras clave – provistas por la editorial
No disponibles.
Disponibilidad
Institución detectada | Período | Navegá | Descargá | Solicitá |
---|
Información
Tipo de recurso:
revistas
ISSN impreso
1092-4388
País de edición
Estados Unidos
Fecha de publicación
1997-
Cobertura temática
Tabla de contenidos
Gauging the Auditory Dimensions of Dysarthric Impairment: Reliability and Construct Validity of the Bogenhausen Dysarthria Scales (BoDyS)
Wolfram Ziegler; Anja Staiger; Theresa Schölderle; Mathias Vogel
<jats:sec> <jats:title>Purpose</jats:title> <jats:p>Standardized clinical assessment of dysarthria is essential for management and research. We present a new, fully standardized dysarthria assessment, the Bogenhausen Dysarthria Scales (BoDyS). The measurement model of the BoDyS is based on auditory evaluations of connected speech using 9 scales (traits) assessed by 4 elicitation methods. Analyses of the BoDyS' reliability and construct validity were performed to test this model, with the aim of gauging the auditory dimensions of speech impairment in dysarthria.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Method</jats:title> <jats:p>Interrater agreement was examined in 70 persons with dysarthria. Construct validity was examined in 190 persons with dysarthria using a multitrait-multimethod design with confirmatory factor analysis.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Results</jats:title> <jats:p>Interrater agreement of < 1 on a 5-point scale was found in 91% of cases across listener pairs and scales. Average reliability was .85. Inspection of the multitrait-multimethod matrix pointed at a high convergent and discriminant validity. Modeling of the BoDyS trait and method factors using confirmatory factor analysis yielded high goodness of fit. Model coefficients confirmed high discriminant and convergent validity and revealed meaningful relationships between scales and methods.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Conclusions</jats:title> <jats:p>The 9 auditory scales of the BoDyS provide a reliable and valid profile of dysarthric impairment. They permit standardized measurement of clinically relevant dimensions of dysarthric speech.</jats:p> </jats:sec>
Palabras clave: Speech and Hearing; Linguistics and Language; Language and Linguistics.
Pp. 1516-1534
Identification and Remediation of Phonological and Motor Errors in Acquired Sound Production Impairment
Adam Buchwald; Bernadine Gagnon; Michele Miozzo
<jats:sec> <jats:title>Purpose</jats:title> <jats:p>This study aimed to test whether an approach to distinguishing errors arising in phonological processing from those arising in motor planning also predicts the extent to which repetition-based training can lead to improved production of difficult sound sequences.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Method</jats:title> <jats:p>Four individuals with acquired speech production impairment who produced consonant cluster errors involving deletion were examined using a repetition task. We compared the acoustic details of productions with deletion errors in target consonant clusters to singleton consonants. Changes in accuracy over the course of the study were also compared.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Results</jats:title> <jats:p>Two individuals produced deletion errors consistent with a phonological locus of the errors, and 2 individuals produced errors consistent with a motoric locus of the errors. The 2 individuals who made phonologically driven errors showed no change in performance on a repetition training task, whereas the 2 individuals with motoric errors improved in their production of both trained and untrained items.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Conclusions</jats:title> <jats:p>The results extend previous findings about a metric for identifying the source of sound production errors in individuals with both apraxia of speech and aphasia. In particular, this work may provide a tool for identifying predominant error types in individuals with complex deficits.</jats:p> </jats:sec>
Palabras clave: Speech and Hearing; Linguistics and Language; Language and Linguistics.
Pp. 1726-1738
Technology-Enhanced Reading Therapy for People With Aphasia: Findings From a Quasirandomized Waitlist Controlled Study
Anna Caute; Celia Woolf; Stephanie Wilson; Carol Stokes; Katie Monnelly; Madeline Cruice; Katherine Bacon; Jane Marshall
<jats:sec> <jats:title>Purpose</jats:title> <jats:p>This study investigated the effects of technology-enhanced reading therapy for people with reading impairments, using mainstream assistive reading technologies alongside reading strategies.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Method</jats:title> <jats:p>The study used a quasirandomized waitlist controlled design. Twenty-one people with reading impairments following stroke were randomly assigned to receive 14 hr of therapy immediately or after a 6-week delay. During therapy, participants were trained to use assistive reading technology that offered a range of features to support reading comprehension. They developed skills in using the technology independently and in applying the technology to their personal reading goals. The primary outcome measure assessed reading comprehension, using Gray Oral Reading Test–Fourth Edition (GORT-4). Secondary measures were as follows: Reading Comprehension Battery for Aphasia–Second Edition, Reading Confidence and Emotions Questionnaire, Communication Activities of Daily Living–Second Edition, Visual Analog Mood Scales, and Assessment of Living With Aphasia. Matched texts were used with the GORT-4 to compare technology-assisted and unassisted reading comprehension. Mixed analyses of variance explored change between T1 and T2, when the immediate group had received therapy but the delayed group had not, thus serving as untreated controls. Pretherapy, posttherapy, and follow-up scores on the measures were also examined for all participants.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Results</jats:title> <jats:p>GORT-4 results indicated that the immediately treated group improved significantly in technology-assisted reading following therapy, but not in unassisted reading. However, the data were not normally distributed, and secondary nonparametric analysis was not significant. The control group was unstable over the baseline, improving significantly in unassisted reading. The whole-group analysis showed significant gains in assisted (but not unassisted) reading after therapy that were maintained at follow-up. The Reading Confidence and Emotions Questionnaire results improved significantly following therapy, with good maintenance of change. Results on all other secondary measures were not significant.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Conclusions</jats:title> <jats:p>Technology-assisted reading comprehension improved following the intervention, with treatment compensating for, rather than remediating, the reading impairment. Participants' confidence and emotions associated with reading also improved. Gains were achieved after 14 therapy sessions, using assistive technologies that are widely available and relatively affordable, meaning that this approach could be implemented in clinical practice.</jats:p> </jats:sec>
Palabras clave: Speech and Hearing; Linguistics and Language; Language and Linguistics.
Pp. 4382-4416
Improved Conversation Outcomes After Social Communication Skills Training for People With Traumatic Brain Injury and Their Communication Partners: A Clinical Trial Investigating In-Person and Telehealth Delivery
Rachael Rietdijk; Emma Power; Michelle Attard; Robert Heard; Leanne Togher
<jats:sec> <jats:title>Purpose</jats:title> <jats:p>The aim of the study was to investigate the effectiveness of social communication skills training (TBIconneCT) for people with traumatic brain injury (TBI) and their communication partners, delivered in-person or via telehealth, on quality of conversations.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Method</jats:title> <jats:p> This study is a clinical trial, including an in-person intervention group ( <jats:italic>n =</jats:italic> 17), a telehealth intervention group ( <jats:italic>n =</jats:italic> 19), and a historical control group ( <jats:italic>n =</jats:italic> 15). Participants were adults at least 6 months post moderate-to-severe TBI with social communication skills deficits and their usual communication partners. Participants completed a casual and purposeful conversation task at pre-intervention, postintervention, and a follow-up assessment. A blinded assessor evaluated conversations using the Adapted Measure of Participation in Conversation and the Adapted Measure of Support in Conversation. Treatment effects were examined by comparing groups on change in ratings between pre- and posttraining. Maintenance of effects was examined using change between posttraining and follow-up assessment. The trial protocol was registered with the Australian New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry (ACTRN12615001024538). </jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Results</jats:title> <jats:p>Trained participants with TBI had significant improvements in participation in casual conversation compared to controls. Trained communication partners also had significant improvements compared to controls on ratings of support in casual conversations. However, treatment effects were not maintained at follow-up for two of eight measures. Comparisons between outcomes of in-person and telehealth groups found negligible to small effect sizes for six of eight measures.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Conclusions</jats:title> <jats:p>The findings reinforce previous studies demonstrating the efficacy of communication partner training after TBI. Telehealth delivery produced similar outcomes to in-person delivery.</jats:p> </jats:sec>
Palabras clave: Speech and Hearing; Linguistics and Language; Language and Linguistics.
Pp. 615-632
Which Measures Better Discriminate Language Minority Bilingual Children With and Without Developmental Language Disorder? A Study Testing a Combined Protocol of First and Second Language Assessment
Paola Bonifacci; Elena Atti; Martina Casamenti; Barbara Piani; Marina Porrelli; Rita Mari
<jats:sec> <jats:title>Purpose</jats:title> <jats:p>This study aimed to assess a protocol for the evaluation of developmental language disorder (DLD) in language minority bilingual children (LMBC). The specific aims were (a) to test group differences, (b) to evaluate the discriminant validity of single measures included in the protocol, and (c) to define which model of combined variables had the best results in terms of efficacy and efficiency.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Method</jats:title> <jats:p> Two groups of LMBC were involved, one with typical development ( <jats:italic>n</jats:italic> = 35) selected from mainstream schools and one with DLD ( <jats:italic>n</jats:italic> = 20). The study protocol included the collection of demographic information and linguistic history; a battery of standardized tests in their second language (Italian), including nonword repetition, morphosyntactic comprehension and production, and vocabulary and narrative skills; and direct (children's evaluation) and indirect (parents' questionnaire) assessment of linguistic skills in their first language. </jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Results</jats:title> <jats:p>Results showed that the two groups differed in almost all linguistic measures. None of the single measures reached good specificity/sensitivity scores. A combined model that included direct and indirect assessment of first language skills, morphosyntactic comprehension and production, and nonword repetition reached good discriminant validity, with 94.5% of cases correctly classified.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Discussion</jats:title> <jats:p>The study defines a complex picture of the linguistic profile in bilingual children with DLD, compared to typically developing bilingual peers. The results reinforce the idea that no single measure can be considered optimal in distinguishing children with DLD from typical peers. The study offers a concrete example of an effective and efficient protocol with which to discriminate LMBC with and without DLD.</jats:p> </jats:sec>
Palabras clave: Speech and Hearing; Linguistics and Language; Language and Linguistics.
Pp. 1898-1915
Outcomes for Feeding Tube-Dependent Children With Oral Aversion in an Intensive Interdisciplinary Treatment Program
Nancy F. Bandstra; Parker L. Huston; Kate Zvonek; Carly Heinz; Emily Piccione
<jats:sec> <jats:title>Purpose</jats:title> <jats:p>Feeding challenges in children are common, at times reaching a severity that requires the placement and long-term use of enteral feedings. A significant barrier to advancing the oral eating of some tube-dependent children is the presence of oral aversion. Although some research exists regarding the treatment of tube-dependent children who are averse to food or the process of eating, specifically, there has yet to be an examination of children who are truly “orally” averse—resisting not just the presentation of food or liquid but also nonnutritive stimuli presented extra- or intra-orally.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Method</jats:title> <jats:p> Using a retrospective chart review, the current study aimed to examine the treatment outcomes of 18 feeding tube-dependent children with significant oral aversion (nine boys, nine girls; <jats:italic>M</jats:italic> <jats:sub>age</jats:sub> = 46.7 months, <jats:italic>SD</jats:italic> = 20.0 months, range: 11.4–89.3 months) as compared to 29 tube-dependent, but nonorally averse, clinical controls. Children completed approximately 6–8 weeks of intensive interdisciplinary feeding treatment. </jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Results</jats:title> <jats:p>Analyses revealed significant improvements in all measured treatment outcomes for both patient groups, including children's rates of acceptance and maladaptive mealtime behavior. Significant reductions in tube use were also observed across both groups, with tube utilization decreasing, on average, by 76.2% for orally averse and by 64.3% for nonorally averse children by program discharge.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Conclusion</jats:title> <jats:p>Results demonstrate the therapeutic benefits of intensive interdisciplinary intervention for both groups of tube-dependent children, highlighting that orally averse children, believed to be an especially challenging subset of pediatric feeding patients, demonstrate similarly positive treatment responses. Interestingly, in this preliminary examination, orally averse children demonstrated significantly greater reductions in tube utilization following discharge when compared with their nonorally averse clinical peers.</jats:p> </jats:sec>
Palabras clave: Speech and Hearing; Linguistics and Language; Language and Linguistics.
Pp. 2497-2507
Young Adults With Developmental Language Disorder: A Systematic Review of Education, Employment, and Independent Living Outcomes
Pascale Dubois; Marie-Catherine St-Pierre; Chantal Desmarais; Frédéric Guay
<jats:sec> <jats:title>Purpose</jats:title> <jats:p>Research on developmental language disorder (DLD) in adulthood has increased rapidly in recent years. However, to date, there has been no systematic literature review on this topic, thereby limiting the possibility to have a comprehensive overview of publications in this field.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Method</jats:title> <jats:p>Following Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines, we conducted a systematic literature review. A literature search was undertaken in four databases, from 2005 to 2018. We selected articles with original data related to life outcomes of young adults with and without DLD, all aged between 18 and 34 years, in three life areas: education, employment, and independent living. Methodological characteristics of the studies were analyzed.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Results</jats:title> <jats:p>Fifteen articles were selected with longitudinal designs. In every life area, young adults with DLD were compared to their typically developing peers to identify their strengths and weaknesses. The predictive role of language abilities was also examined.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Conclusions</jats:title> <jats:p>Outcomes within each life area are heterogeneous. Nevertheless, similarly to young children and adolescents, young adults with DLD face numerous challenges. Although language abilities partly predict some of these outcomes, much of the variance remains unaccounted for and some outcomes are unrelated to this predictor. This systematic literature review has implications for researchers and practitioners to identify promising avenues for research, interventions, and policy development.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Supplemental Material</jats:title> <jats:p> <jats:ext-link xmlns:xlink="http://www.w3.org/1999/xlink" ext-link-type="uri" xlink:href="https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.13022552">https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.13022552</jats:ext-link> </jats:p> </jats:sec>
Palabras clave: Speech and Hearing; Linguistics and Language; Language and Linguistics.
Pp. 3786-3800
Emergent Literacy in Spanish-Speaking Children With Developmental Language Disorder: Preliminary Findings of Delays in Comprehension- and Code-Related Skills
Amy S. Pratt; John A. Grinstead; Rebecca J. McCauley
<jats:sec> <jats:title>Purpose</jats:title> <jats:p>This exploratory study describes the emergent literacy skills of children with developmental language disorder (DLD) who speak Spanish, a language with a simple phonological structure and transparent orthography. We examine differences between children with DLD and their typically developing (TD) peers on a battery of emergent literacy measures.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Method</jats:title> <jats:p> Participants included 15 monolingual Spanish-speaking children with DLD (who did not present with cognitive difficulties) and 15 TD controls matched for age, gender, and socioeconomic status, ranging in age from 3;10 to 6;6 (years;months; <jats:italic>M</jats:italic> <jats:sub>age</jats:sub> = 4;11). All children completed a battery of comprehension-related emergent literacy tasks (narrative retell, print concept knowledge) and code-related emergent literacy tasks (beginning sound, rhyming awareness, alphabet knowledge, and name-writing ability). </jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Results</jats:title> <jats:p>On average, children with DLD performed significantly worse than TD controls on a battery of comprehension- and code-related emergent literacy measures. On all code-related skills except rhyming, children with DLD were more likely than their TD peers to score “at risk.”</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Conclusions</jats:title> <jats:p>The results suggest some universality in the effect of DLD on reading development. Difficulties with emergent literacy that are widely documented in English-speaking children with DLD were similarly observed in Spanish-speaking children with DLD. Future research should explore long-term reading outcomes in Spanish for children with DLD.</jats:p> </jats:sec>
Palabras clave: Speech and Hearing; Linguistics and Language; Language and Linguistics.
Pp. 4193-4207
Development of an Auditory Passage Comprehension Task for Swedish Primary School Children of Cultural and Linguistic Diversity
Johanna Carlie; Birgitta Sahlén; Jens Nirme; Ketty Andersson; Mary Rudner; Roger Johansson; Agneta Gulz; K. Jonas Brännström
<jats:sec> <jats:title>Purpose</jats:title> <jats:p>This study reports on the development of an auditory passage comprehension task for Swedish primary school children of cultural and linguistic diversity. It also reports on their performance on the task in quiet and in noise.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Method</jats:title> <jats:p>Eighty-eight children aged 7–9 years and showing normal hearing participated. The children were divided into three groups based on presumed language exposure: 13 children were categorized as Swedish-speaking monolinguals, 19 children were categorized as simultaneous bilinguals, and 56 children were categorized as sequential bilinguals. No significant difference in working memory capacity was seen between the three language groups. Two passages and associated multiple-choice questions were developed. During development of the passage comprehension task, steps were taken to reduce the impact of culture-specific prior experience and knowledge on performance. This was achieved by using the story grammar principles, universal topics and plots, and simple language that avoided complex or unusual grammatical structures and words.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Results</jats:title> <jats:p>The findings indicate no significant difference between the two passages and similar response distributions. Passage comprehension performance was significantly better in quiet than in noise, regardless of language exposure group. The monolinguals outperformed both simultaneous and sequential bilinguals in both listening conditions.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Conclusions</jats:title> <jats:p>Because the task was designed to minimize the effect of cultural knowledge on auditory passage comprehension, this suggests that compared with monolinguals, both simultaneous and sequential bilinguals have a disadvantage in auditory passage comprehension. As expected, the findings demonstrate that noise has a negative effect on auditory passage comprehension. The magnitude of this effect does not relate to language exposure. The developed auditory passage comprehension task seems suitable for assessing auditory passage comprehension in primary school children of linguistic and cultural diversity.</jats:p> </jats:sec>
Palabras clave: Speech and Hearing; Linguistics and Language; Language and Linguistics.
Pp. 1-11
A Systematic Review of Expressive and Receptive Prosody in People With Dementia
Chorong Oh; Richard J. Morris; Xianhui Wang
<jats:sec> <jats:title>Purpose</jats:title> <jats:p>This review was designed to provide a systematic overview of prosody in people with a primary diagnosis of dementia (PwD) and evaluate the potential use of prosodic features for diagnosis of dementia.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Method</jats:title> <jats:p>A systematic search of five databases was conducted using Medical Subject Headings and keywords. Studies included in the review were evaluated for their methodological quality using the modified Joanna Briggs Institute checklist.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Results</jats:title> <jats:p>A total of 14 articles were identified as being relevant for this review. Among the 14 articles, the methodological quality ranged, with eight rated as weak, four rated as moderate, and two rated as strong. Ten of the 14 articles had people with Alzheimer's disease (AD) as participants, and the remaining four had people with frontotemporal dementia as participants. Four articles focused on receptive prosody, another six focused on expressive prosody, and the remaining four articles were investigations into both. The 14 articles presented inconsistent findings, and various tasks were used to measure prosodic features in PwD in the articles. Prosody was studied as a diagnostic tool for dementia in four of the articles, all of which were based on expressive prosody in individuals with AD. Among the four articles, three proposed the use of automatic speech analysis for diagnosis of AD.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Conclusions</jats:title> <jats:p>This review demonstrates that prosody in PwD is an underinvestigated area. In particular, it was concerning that most articles were of weak methodological quality. Nevertheless, it was found that prosody may be a potential diagnostic tool for assessing dementia. More studies that replicate the existing studies and those with stronger methodology are needed to confirm that receptive and/or expressive prosody can be used for dementia diagnosis.</jats:p> </jats:sec>
Palabras clave: Speech and Hearing; Linguistics and Language; Language and Linguistics.
Pp. 1-23